Planned behaviour – stagnation or evolution?

Tom St Quinton looks at the classic theory and alternatives in the latest 'New voices'.

04 November 2016

There it was; I had it. To change behaviour one has to alter a person’s intention. If an individual doesn’t hold an intention to engage in a specific behaviour, then the chances are they will not do it. Furthermore, to change these intentions, we have to ‘motivate’ them, perhaps by offering the advantages or convincing them that it is achievable. When you do this, given the right control conditions, it is likely that an individual will succeed in enacting their originally stated intention.

OK, so perhaps it’s not the most exciting of models. But the theory of planned behaviour (TPB: Ajzen, 1985) has contributed much to psychological advances, none more so than restoring the attitude–behaviour relation. The fact a behavioural scientist can easily highlight and attempt to alter the relevant underlying beliefs is a positive, as are its parsimonious variables.

Despite this, the criticisms have kept on coming. Although some are valid, others seem unfair. As Head and Noar (2014) suggest, it appears that the theory is stuck between issues concerning generalisability and utility. Gollwitzer and Oettingen (2015) offer the analogy of borrowing a newly invented race bike that the creator went to great lengths to produce. After taking it out on the mountains, it breaks. Hence, there is anger both on the side of the creator (for the damage) and the borrower (poor usability). But the creator stressed that it was a general bike, one of the first in fact. ‘Why did you take it up there!’ he bellows. Likening this to health psychology, the theory has been applied to almost all behaviours, ranging from colonoscopy appointments to engaging in physical activity, and the results have been mixed. Surprised that a parsimonious model including only four determinants has failed to successfully change each and every behaviour? Perhaps not. It appears theorists strive for generalisability and practitioners for utility. Nevertheless, accounting for various moderating effects, the theory has proved efficient in explaining a wide range of behaviours (McEachan et al., 2011).  

Although interventions based on the theory have shown limited success (Hardeman et al., 2002), it appears that the absence of strategies or techniques for change are attributed to a deficiency in the theory. As a model of prediction and explanation rather than change, the theory may indeed lack sufficient belief-alteration guidelines, but this evidence in itself isn’t sufficient to critique the theory. It addresses a different concern altogether, that of intervention design. Poor use of the theory, rather than the theory being poor, leads to a large proportion of negative intervention findings.

With falsifiability an imperative of science (Popper, 1959), concerns regarding the TPB’s exploration of analytic truths have been raised (Ogden, 2003). What if it cannot be empirically disproved, and null hypotheses findings are simply attributed to methodological failures? Sniehotta and colleagues (2014) argue this in their paper ‘Time to retire the theory of planned behaviour’ (although oddly they use experimental findings supporting the null hypothesis – e.g Sniehotta, 2009 – to justify these claims).

What does Ajzen himself say? He feels (2015) there are many flaws in the application of the theory that subsequently led to ineffective interventions, and has suggested (1991) that a determinant shouldn’t be introduced unless it offers more variance than the others already included. The inclusion of perceived behavioural control from the theory of reasoned action (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980) offers a fine example here. He also argues that falsifiability can be achieved by demonstrating a limited effect of a determinant on the outcome variable. The contribution of the determinants has indeed been mixed: for example, attitudes and perceived behavioural control have been found to influence intentions more than the subjective norm (SN) in certain behaviours, whereas in others SN has been more salient. The issue of generalisability again raises its head. It appears that the TPB’s main strength, its parsimony, is also its major limitation.

But where does that leave the health scientists? Although there has been an enormous amount of literature regarding health behaviours, it is questionable whether there has been progression (Noar & Zimmerman, 2005). Could it be, though, that literature simply gets lost in the plethora of published articles? Head and Noar (2014) suggest that this may result in researchers struggling to keep up with relevant literature and to then conclude that rather than theoretical evolution, stagnation has occurred. Perhaps there is more that can be done other than changing exogenous variables. Such theorising has been conducted in a variety of ways, and may be putting ‘excitement’ back into health psychology.

Models such as the health action process approach (HAPA: Schwarzer, 2008) and the integrated behaviour change model for physical activity (Hagger et al., 2002) involve the integration of theoretical ideas and/or determinants from various frameworks. For example, the latter incorporates ideas from self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985) to understand how organismic approaches affect beliefs. Behavioural beliefs can also be understood in terms of their origin, specifically whether it is self- or externally determined. The HAPA encompasses self-efficacy from Bandura’s (1998) theorising, as well as planning strategies. The model explicitly states a motivational and volitional stage, with different interventions required for those not intending and intending to perform the behaviour.

This hybrid approach also differentiates further between types of planning and types of self-efficacy that can act as both mediators and moderators. For example, action planning could transfer positive intentions to successful change if a high level of self-efficacy is present. Despite the benefits of distinguishing between intention formation and behavioural enactment, as it stands, specific applications of the model remain sparse, particularly concerning the development and implementation of practical interventions.

Ego-depletion
The strength model of self-control attempts to explain why individuals fail to overcome urges and impulses and subsequently engage in behaviours that are detrimental. Baumeister and colleagues (2007) liken one’s inability to self-regulate to a muscle that has become worn out, resulting in ego depletion. During this state, individuals are less likely to perform more productive behaviours that they would have done with a fully resourced muscle. Not only is a muscle offered as a metaphor but a physiological explanation is also given, specifically by relating depletion to a decrease in glucose. Whereby, replenishing levels of glucose is vital, as is ensuring that the finite resource is not drained and that desires are not suppressed. This can be achieved in numerous ways such as using one’s weaker hand, making fewer decisions, and engaging in simpler tasks.

Despite initial support, the model has come under criticism. For example, it has been suggested that a lack of self-control could result from other processes, aside from glucose depletion (Inzlicht & Schmeichel, 2012). Bringing behaviour back to the cognitive, it could be simply that a lack of efficacious beliefs results in the transfer failure of intentions into behaviour. Following a similar subjective perception-behaviour link as found in the theory of locus of control (Rotter, 1966), Job et al. (2015) suggest that rather than the depleted resource itself affecting behaviour, merely believing in a finite resource impairs performance.

Nevertheless, the theory offers an alternative explanation for the intention–behaviour gap, one that is not cognitive. This implicates different strategies for intervention aside from the strength and content of intentions. Despite being in its infancy, research should continue to examine the moderating variables that highlight the situations where ego depletion is affected. Motivational cognitive theories view behavioural failure as a resultant of beliefs, attitudes and conscious processes. However, others suggest that behaviour is a result of non-conscious processes, those that the individual is unaware of (Bargh, 1997). Recent approaches have attempted to draw the two apparently separate processes together. Baumeister and Bargh (2014) suggest that the unconscious is the primary contributing factor to behaviour but, nevertheless, relies on consciousness to offer directions and to facilitate in regulation. For example, consciousness can be used to envisage future thoughts, and the unconscious to enact the behaviour. The role played by consciousness is thus increased from that of being just a mere bystander.

The work of Gollwitzer (1999) concerning implementation intentions can be offered as an example. One could consciously envisage performing physical activity and subsequently plan to do so. Following this conscious process, the automated cue then transfers these positive intentions into behaviour. Thus, the activation of the unconscious via the external cue decreases the intention–behaviour gap. Although being the main driver in change, the unconscious cannot operate wholly independently and so it enlists the assistance of the conscious. The automatic effects of planning can also override the self-regulatory effects of ego-depletion (Webb & Sheeran, 2003). Underresearched models following a similar trail of thought could prove fruitful in gaining a better understanding of health behaviour change. For example, interventions applying the recently developed temporal self-regulation theory (Hall & Fong, 2007) should examine the moderating effect of executive function and behavioural prepotency on intention.

Future directions
In summary, intentional models such as the TPB have provided the groundwork for potentially more effective interventions. The TPB can and should be used with those who are not motivated, specifically to ‘kick-start’ the change process. The model offers avenues to inform intervention design, but it is crucial that the formative research is undertaken. Although a move away from cross-sectional studies to more experimental research should be encouraged, specifically using randomised control designs, this would only prove effective if the initial work is done.

Despite this, postintentional models offer a further route of research, which doesn’t aim to alter the content of the intention or strengthen the ‘will’. Self-regulatory skills, planning strategies, unconscious pursuits, and phased-specific self-efficacy could all be used to foster change. With all of these different approaches, it is unlikely that psychologists will sing from the same hymn sheet. But it is important to move between different fields and appreciate new ideas. The process of research can be conducted both ‘slow’ and ‘fast’ simultaneously; the former to ensure accuracy and the latter to ultimately reduce mortality and morbidity. Although the relationship between exciting and effective isn’t one of causation, it is possible for research to have both.

Tom St Quinton is a Graduate Teaching Assistant and PhD student in the Department of Sport, Health & Nutrition
Leeds Trinity University
[email protected]

Reading
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to action: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Ed.) Action control: From cognitions to behaviours (pp.11–39). New York: Springer.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behaviour. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
Ajzen, I. (2015). The theory of planned behaviour is alive and well, and not ready to retire. Health Psychology Review, 9, 131–137.
Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A. (1998). Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychology and Health, 13, 623–649.
Bargh, J.A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R.S. Wyer Jr (Ed.) Advances in social cognition (Vol.10, pp.1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Baumeister, R.F. & Bargh, J.A. (2014). Conscious and unconscious. In J. Sherman, B. Gawronski & Y. Trope (Eds.) Dual process theories of the social mind (pp.33–49) New York: Guilford Press.
Baumeister, R.F., Vohs, K.D. & Tice, D.M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 351–355.
Deci, E.L. & Ryan, R.M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
Gollwitzer, P.M. (1999). Implementation intentions. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.
Gollwitzer, P.M. & Oettingen, G. (2015). From studying the determinants of action to analysing its regulation. Health Psychology Review, 9, 146–150.
Hagger, M.S., Chatzisarantis, N. & Biddle, S.J.H. (2002). A meta-analysis review of the theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour in physical activity. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 3–23.
Hall, P.A. & Fong, G.T. (2007). Temporal self-regulation theory. Health Psychology Review, 1, 6–52.
Hardeman, W., Johnston, M., Johnston, D.W. et al. (2002). Application of the theory of planned behaviour in behaviour change interventions. Psychology and Health, 17, 123–158.
Head, K.J. & Noar, S.M. (2014). Facilitating progress in health behaviour theory development and modification. Health Psychology Review, 8, 34–52.
Inzlicht, M. & Schmeichel, B.J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 450–463.
Job, V., Bernecker, K., Miketta, S. & Friese, M. (2015). Implicit theories about willpower predict the activation of a rest goal following self-control exertion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 694–706.
McEachan, R.R.C., Conner, M., Taylor, N.J. & Lawton, R.J. (2011). Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review, 5, 97–144.
Noar, S.M. & Zimmerman, R.S. (2005). Health behaviour theory and cumulative knowledge regarding health behaviors. Health Education Research, 20, 275–290.
Ogden, J. (2003). Some problems with social cognition models: A pragmatic and conceptual analysis. Health Psychology, 22, 424–428.
Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.
Rotter, J.B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80, 1–28.
Schwarzer, R. (2008). Modeling health behavior change. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 57, 1–29.
Sniehotta, F. (2009). An experimental test of the theory of planned behavior. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 1, 257–270.
Sniehotta, F.F., Presseau, J. & Araújo-Soares, V. (2014). Time to retire the theory of planned behavior. Health Psychology Review, 8, 1–7.
Webb, T.L. & Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation intentions help to overcome ego depletion? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 279–286.